Management
Abernethy, B., Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R.S.W., Van Der Kamp, J., & Jackson, R. C. (2007). Attentional processes in skill learning and expert performance. In G. Tennenbaum & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (3nd ed., pp. 245–263). New York, NY: Wiley.
Allsop, J. E., Lawrence, G. P., Gray, R., & Kahn, M. A. (2017). The interaction between practice and performance pressure on the planning and control of fast target directed movement. Psychological Research, 81, 1004-1019. doi: 10.1007/s00426-016-0791-0
Amanvermez, Y., Rahmadiana, M., Karyotaki, E., de Wit, L., Ebert, D. D., Kessler, R. C., & Cuijpers, P. (2023). Stress management interventions for college students: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 30(4), 423–444. https://doi.org/10.1111/cpsp.12342
Anderson, B. A. (2021). An adaptive view of attentional control. American Psychologist, 76, 1416-1422.
Baddeley, A. D. (2001). Is working memory still working? American Psychologist, 56, 851–864.
Bakker, F. C., Oudejans, R. R. D., Binsch, O., & Van der Kamp, J. (2006). Penalty shooting and gaze behavior: Unwanted effects of the wish not to miss. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 37, 265–280.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy. The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman.
Bartholomay, E. M., Stone, B. M., Koran, J., Björgvinsson, T., Kertz, S. J. (2023). Repetitive negative thinking explains the relationship between perceived attentional control and generalized anxiety symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 45(1), 181-193. doi: 10.1007/s10862-022-09997-1
Baumeister, R. F. (1984). Choking under pressure: Self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 610–620.
Baumeister, R. F. (1995). Disputing the effects of championship pressures and home audiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 644–648.
Baumeister, R. F., Hamilton, J. C., & Tice, D. M. (1985). Public versus private expectancy of success: Confidence booster or performance pressure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1447–1457.
Baumeister, R. F., & Showers, C. J. (1986). A review of paradoxical performance effects: Choking under pressure in sports and mental tests. European Journal of Social Psychology, 16, 361–383.
Baumeister, R. F., & Steinhilber, A. (1984). Paradoxical effects of supportive audiences on performance under pressure: The home field disadvantage in sports championships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 85–93.
Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Hutton, D. G. (1989). Self-presentation motivations and personality differences in self-esteem. Journal of Personality, 57, 547–579.
Baumeister, R. F., Twenge, J. M., & Nuss, C. K. (2002). Effects of social exclusion on cognitive processes: Anticipated aloneness reduces intelligent thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 817–827.
Bawden, M., & Maynard, I. (2001). Towards an understanding of the personal experience of the “yips” in cricketers. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19, 937–953.
Beckmann, J., Gröpel, P., Ehrlenspiel, F. (2013). Preventing motor skill failure through hemisphere-specific priming: Cases from choking under pressure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 679-691. doi: 10.1037/a0029852
Beilock, S. L., Afremow, J. A., Rabe, A. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). “Don’t miss!” The debilitating effects of suppressive imagery on golf putting performance. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 23, 200–221.
Beilock, S. L., Bertenthal, B. I., McCoy, A. M., & Carr, T. H. (2004). Haste does not always make waste: Expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 373–379.
Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 40, 701–725.
Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2005). When high-powered people fail. Working Memory and “choking under pressure” in math. Psychological Science, 16, 101–105.
Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16.
Beilock, S. L., & Gonzo, S. (2008). Putting in mind versus putting on the green: Expertise, performance time, and the linking of imagery and action. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 920–932. doi: 10.1080/17470210701625626
Beilock, S. L., & Gray, R. (2007). Why do athletes choke under pressure? In G. Tenenbaum & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (3rd ed., pp. 425– 444). New York, NY: Wiley.
Beilock, S. L., Jellison, W. A., Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., & Carr, T. H. (2006). On the causal mechanisms of stereotype threat: Can skills that don’t rely heavily on working memory still be threatened? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1059–1071.
Beilock, S. L., Kulp, C. A., Holt, L. E., & Carr, T. H. (2004). More on the fragility of performance: Choking under pressure in mathematical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 584–600. doi: 10.1037/0096–3445.133.4.584
Beilock, S. L., & McConnell, A. R. (2004). Stereotype treat and sport: Can athletic performance be threatened? Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 26, 597–609.
Beilock, S. L., Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R. (2007). Stereotype threat and working memory: Mechanisms, alleviation, and spillover. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 256–276.
Bell, J. L., & Hardy, J. (2009). Effects of attentional focus on skilled performance in golf. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21, 163–177.
Bell, R. L., Skinner, C. H., & Fisher, L. A. (2009). Decreasing putting yips in accomplished golfers via solution-focused guided imagery: A single-subject research design. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21, 1–14.
Belletier, C., Davanche, K., Tellier, I. S., Dumas, F., Vidal, F., Hasbroucz, T., & Huguet, P. (2015). Choking under monitoring pressure: being watched by the experimenter reduces executive attention. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 22, 1410-1416. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0804-9
Ben-Zeev, T., Fein, S., & Inzlicht, M. (2005). Arousal and stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 174–181.
Bernier, M., Codron, R., Thienot, E., & Fournier, J. F. (2011). The attentional focus of expert golfers in training and competition: A naturalistic investigation. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 23, 1153–1571. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2011.561518
Berry, D. C., & Dienes, Z. (1993). Implicit learning: Theoretical and empirical issues. Hove, England: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Brown, T. D. Jr., VanRaalte, J. L., Brewer, B. W., Winter, C. R., Cornelius, A. E., & Andersen, M. B. (2002). World Cup soccer home advantage. Journal of Sport Behavior, 25, 134–144.
Burton, D., & Naylor, S. (1997). Is anxiety really facilitative? Reaction to the myth that cognitive anxiety always impairs sport performance. Journal of Applied and Sport Psychology, 9, 295–302.
Butler, J. L., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The trouble with friendly faces: Skilled performance with a supportive audience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1213–1230.
Caliari, P. (2008). Enhancing forehand acquisition in table tennis: The role of mental practice. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 20, 88–96.
Carver, C. S. (1996). Cognitive interference and the structure of behavior. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference theories, methods, and findings (pp. 25–45). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Carver, C. S., & Baird, E. (1998). The American dream revisited: Is it what you want or why you want it that matters? Psychological Science, 9, 289–292.
Carver, C. S., Lawrence, J. W., & Scheier, M. F. (1996). A control-process perspective on the origins of affect. In L. M. Martin, & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling. Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 11–52). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1992). Confidence, doubt and coping with anxiety. In D. G. Forgays, T. Sosnowski, & K Wresiewski (Eds.), Anxiety: Recent developments in cognitive, psychophysiological and health research (pp. 13–22). London, England: Hemisphere.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). A control-process perspective on anxiety. Anxiety Research, 1, 17–22.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2011). Self-regulation of action and affect. In K. K. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation research, theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 3–21). New York, NY: Guilford.
Castaneda, B., & Gray, R. (2007). Effects of focus of attention on baseball batting performance in players of differing skill levels. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 29, 60–77.
Causer, J., Holmes, P. S., Smith, N. C., & Williams, A. M. (2011). Anxiety, movement kinematics, and visual attention in elite-level performers. Emotion, 11, 595–602. doi: 10.1037/a0023225
Chalabaev, A., Serrazin, P., Stone, J., & Cury, F. (2008). Do achievement goals mediate stereotype threat? An investigation on females’ soccer performance. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 30, 143–158.
Cheng, W-N. K., Hardy, L., & Markland, D. (2009). Toward a three-dimensional conceptualization of performance anxiety: Rationale and initial measurement development. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 271–278. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2008.08.001
Cheryan, S., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). When positive stereotypes threaten intellectual performance: The psychological hazards of “model minority” status. Psychological Science, 11, 399–402.
Chin, B., Lindsay, E. K., Greco, C. M., Brown, K. W., Smyth, J. M., Wright, A. G. C., & Creswell, J. D. (2021). Mindfulness interventions improve momentary and trait measures of attentional control: Evidence from a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(4), 686-699. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000969
Clark, R. D. (2002). Do professional golfers “choke”? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94, 1124–1130.
Cogan, K. D. (2019). Coaching Olympic athletes with sport psychology. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, 71(2), 86-96. doi: 10.1037/cpb0000129
Collins, D. V., Jones, B., Fairweather, M., Doolan, S., & Priestley, N. (2001). Examining anxiety associated changes in movement patterns. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 32, 223–242.
Connor J., Woolf J., & Mazanov J. (2013). Would they dope? Revisiting the Goldman dilemma. British Journal of Sports Medicine 47, 697-700.
Conroy, D. E. (2003). Representational models associated with fear of failure in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Personality, 71, 557–783.
Conroy, D. E. (2004). The unique psychological meanings of multidimensional fears of failing. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 26, 484–491.
Conroy, D E. (2008). Fear of failure in the context of competitive sport: A commentary. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 11, 423–432. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2010.04.013
Conroy, D. E., & Coatsworth, J. D. (2007). Coaching behaviors associated with changes in fear of failure: Changes in self-talk and need for satisfaction as potential mechanisms. Journal of Personality, 75, 383–419.
Conroy, D. E., & Elliot, A. J. (2004). Fear of failure and achievement goals in sport: Addressing the issue of the chicken and the egg. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 17, 271–285.
Conroy, D. E., & Metzler, J. N. (2004). Patterns of self-talk associated with different forms of competitive anxiety. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 26, 69–89.
Craft, L. L., Magyar, T. M., Becker, B. J., & Feltz, D. L. (2003). The relationship between the Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2 and sport performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 25, 45–65.
Crosswell, A. D., Mayer, S. E., Whitehurst, L. N., Picard, M., Zebarjadian, S., & Epel, E. S. (2024). Deep rest: An integrative model of how contemplative practices combat stress and enhance the body’s restorative capacity. Psychological Review, 131(1), 247–270. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000453
Daou, M., Hutchinson, Z., Bacelar, M., Rhodes, J. A., Lohse, K. R., & Miller, M. W. (2018). Learning a skill with the expectation of teaching it impairs the skill's execution under psychological pressure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 25, 219-229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xap0000191
DeCaro, M. S., Thomas, R. D., Albert, N. B., & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Choking under pressure: Multiple routes to skill failure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 190-406. doi: 10.1037/a0023466>
Dugdale, J. R., & Eklund, R. C. (2002). Do not pay attention to the empires: Thought suppression and task-relevant focusing strategies. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24, 306–319.
Dunne, S., Chib, V. S., Berleant, J., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2019). Reappraisal of incentives ameliorates choking under pressure and is correlated with changes in the neural representations of incentives. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13-22. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy108
Edwards, T., Kingston, K., Hardy, L., & Gould, D. (2002). A qualitative analysis of catastrophic performances and the associated thoughts, feelings and emotions. The Sport Psychologist, 16, 1–19.
Eklund, R. C. (1994). A season long investigation of competitive cognition in collegiate wrestlers. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 65, 169–183.
Eklund, R. C. (1996). Preparing to compete: A season-long investigation with collegiate wrestlers. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 111–131.
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (1999). Test anxiety and the hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 628–644.
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.
Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409–434.
Eysenck, M. W., & Derakshan, N. (1997). Cognitive biases for future negative events as a function of trait anxiety and social desirability. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 597–605.
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353.
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 101–135.
Ford, P., Hodges, N. J., & Williams, A. M. (2005). Online attentional focus manipulations in a soccer-dribbling task: Implications for the proceduralization of motor skills. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37, 386–394.
Furley, P., Dicks, M., Stendtke, F., & Memmert, D. (2012). “Get it out the way. The wait’s killing me.” Hastening and hiding during soccer penalty kicks. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13, 454–465. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.01.009
Gibson, B., Sachau, D., Doll, B., & Shumate, R. (2002). Sandbagging in competition: Responding to the pressure of being the favorite. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1119–1130.
Gimmig, D., Huguet, P., Caverni, J-P, & Cury, F. (2006). Choking under pressure and working memory capacity: When performance pressure reduces fluid intelligence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 1005–1010.
Giacobbi, P. R., Jr., & Weinberg, R. S. (2000). An examination of coping in sport: Individual trait anxiety differences and situational consistency. Sport Psychologist, 14, 42–62.
Ginty, A. T., Oosterhoff, B. J., Young, D. A., & Williams, S. E. (2022). Effects of arousal reappraisal on the anxiety responses to stress: Breaking the cycle of negative arousal intensity and arousal interpretation. British Journal of Psychology, 113, 131–152. doi:10.1111/bjop.12528
González, J.M., Johnson, F.R., Fedoruk, Posner, J. & Bowers, L. (2018). Trading health risks for glory: A reformulation of the Goldman Dilemma. Sports Medicine 48, 1963–1969. https://doi-org.wcsu.idm.oclc.org/10.1007/s40279-018-0881-9
Gould, D., Dieffenbach, K., & Moffett, A. (2002). Psychological characteristics and their development in Olympic champions. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 172–204.
Gould, D., Eklund, R. C., & Jackson, S. A. (1992a). 1988 U.S. Olympic wrestling excellence: I. Mental preparation, competitive cognition, and affect. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 358–382.
Gould, D., Eklund, R. C., & Jackson, S. A. (1992b). 1988 U.S. Olympic wrestling excellence: II. Thoughts and affect occurring during competition. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 383–402.
Gould, D., Eklund, R. C., & Jackson, S. A. (1993). Coping strategies used by U.S. Olympic wrestlers. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 64, 83–93.
Gould, D., Eklund, R. C., Petlichkoff, L., Peterson, K., & Bump, L. (1991). Psychological predictors of state anxiety and performance in age-group wrestlers. Pediatric Exercise Science, 3, 198–206.
Gould, D., Greenleaf, C., Guinan, D., & Chung, Y. C. (2002). A survey of U.S. Olympic coaches: Variables perceived to have influenced athlete performances and coach effectiveness. The Sport Psychologist, 16, 229–250.
Gray, R. (2004). Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: Differences, choking, and slumps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 42–54.
Graydon, J., & Murphy, T. (1995). The effect of personality on social facilitation whilst performing a sports related task. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 265–267.
Grossbard, J. R., Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., & Cumming, S. P. (2009). Competitive anxiety in young athletes: Differentiating somatic anxiety, worry, and concentration disruption. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 22, 153–166. doi: 10.1080/10615800802020643
Gucciardi, D. F., & Dimmock, J. A. (2008). Choking under pressure in sensorimotor skills: Conscious processing or depleted attentional resources? Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 9, 45–59. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.10.007
Gucciardi, D. F., Longbottom, J-L., Jackson, B., & Dimmock, J. A. (2010). Experienced golfers’ perspectives on choking under pressure. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 32, 61–83.
Hanegby, R., & Tenenbaum, G. (2001). Blame it on the racket: norm-breaking behaviours among junior tennis players. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 2, 117–134.
Hardy, L., & Hutchinson, A. (2007). Effects of performance anxiety on effort and performance in rock climbing: A test of processing efficiency theory. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 20, 147–161.
Hardy, L., Mullen, R., & Martin, N. (2001). Effect of task-relevant cues and state anxiety on motor performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92, 943–946.
Heaton, A. W., & Sigall, H. (1991). Self-consciousness, self-presentation, and performance under pressure: Who chokes, and when? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 175–188.
Higuchi, T., Imanaka, K., & Hatayama, T. (2002). Freezing degrees of freedom under stress: Kinematic evidence of constrained movement strategies. Human Movement Science, 21, 831–846. doi: 10.1016/S0167–9457(02)00174–4
Hill, D. M., Hanton, S., Fleming, S., & Matthews, N. (2009). A re-examination of choking in sport. European Journal of Sport Science, 9, 203–212.
Hill, D. M., Hanton, S., Matthews, N., & Fleming, S. (2010). A qualitative exploration of choking in elite golf. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology, 4, 221–240.
Hill, D. M., Hanton, S., Matthews, N., & Fleming, S. (2011). Alleviation of choking under pressure in elite golf: An action research study. The Sport Psychologist, 25, 465–488.
Hiroaki, M., Maruo, Y., Meyer, A., Hajak, G. (2017). Neural correlates of choking under pressure: Athletes high in sports anxiety monitor errors more when performance is being evaluated. Developmental Neuropsychology, 42, 104-112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2016.1274314
Hsu-N., Liu, K, Chang, S. (2019). Choking under the pressure of competition: A complete statistical investigation of pressure kicks in the NFL, 2000-2017. PLoS ONE, 14, e0214096. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0214096
Humphreys, M. S., & Revelle, W. (1984). Personality, motivation, and performance: A theory of the relationship between individual differences and information processing. Psychological Review, 91, 153–184.
Jackson, R. C. (2003). Pre-performance routine consistency: Temporal analysis of goal kicking in the Rugby Union World Cup. Journal of Sports Sciences, 21, 803–814.
Jackson, R. C., Ashford, K. J., & Norsworthy, G. (2006). Attentional focus, dispositional reinvestment and skilled motor performance under pressure. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 28, 49–68.
Jackson, R. C., & Baker, J. S. (2001). Routines, rituals, and rugby: Case study of a world class goal kicker. The Sport Psychologist, 15, 48–65.
Jackson, R. C., & Mogan, P. (2007). Advance visual information, awareness, and anticipation skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39, 341–351.
Jackson, R. C., & Wilson, R. J. (1999). Using “swing thoughts” to prevent paradoxical performance effects in golf putting. In M. R. Farrally & A. J. Cochran (Eds.), Science and golf III: Proceedings of the 1998 world scientific congress of golf (pp. 166–173). Leeds, England: Human Kinetics.
Jackson, S. A. (1995). Factors influencing the occurrence of flow state in elite athletes. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 7, 138–166.
Jackson, S. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Flow in sports. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Jamieson, J. P., & Harkins, S. G. (2007). Mere effort and stereotype threat performance effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 544–564.
Janelle, C. M. (2002). Anxiety, arousal and visual attention: A mechanistic account of performance variability. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 237–251.
Janelle, C. M., Hillman, C. H., Apparies, R., Murray, N. P., Meili, L., & Hatfield, B. D. (2000). Expertise differences in cortical activity and gaze behavior during rifle shooting. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 22, 167–182.
Janelle, C. M., Singer, R. N., & Williams, A. M. (1999). External distraction and attentional narrowing: Visual search evidence. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 21, 70–91.
Jones, G., & Hanton, S. (1996). Interpretation of competitive anxiety symptoms and goal attainment expectancies. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18, 144–157.
Jones, G., & Hanton, S. (2001). Pre-competitive feeling states and directional anxiety interpretations. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19, 385–395.
Jones, G., Hanton, S., & Connaughton, D. (2002). What is this thing called mental toughness: An investigation of elite sport performers. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 205–218. doi: 10.1080/10413200290103509
Jones, G., Hanton, S., & Connaughton, D. (2007). A framework for mental toughness in the world’s best performers. The Sport Psychologist, 21, 243–264.
Jones, G., & Hanton, S., & Swain, A. B. J. (1994). Intensity and interpretation of anxiety symptoms in elite and non-elite sports performers. Personality & Individual Differences, 17, 657–663.
Jones, G., & Swain, A. (1995). Predispositions to experience debilitative and facilitative anxiety in elite and nonelite performers. The Sport Psychologist, 9, 201–211.
Jones, G., Swain, A., & Cale, A. (1991). Gender differences in precompetition temporal patterning and antecedents of anxiety and self-confidence. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 13, 1–15.
Jones, G., Swain, A. B. J., & Hardy, L. (1993). Intensity and direction dimensions of competitive state anxiety and relationships with performance. Journal of Sport Sciences, 11, 525–532.
Jones, K. A., Smith, N. C., & Holmes, P. S. (2004). Anxiety symptom interpretation and performance predictions in high-anxious, low-anxious and repressor sport performers. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 17, 187–199.
Jones, M. I., & Harwood, C. (2008). Psychological momentum within competitive soccer: Players’ perspectives. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 20, 57–72. doi: 10.1080/10413200701784841
Jordet, G. (2005). Perceptual training in soccer: An imagery intervention study with elite players. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 17, 140–156.
Jordet, G. (2009a). When superstars flop: Public status and choking under pressure in international soccer penalty shootouts. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21, 125–130.
Jordet, G. (2009b). Why do English players fail in soccer penalty shootouts? A study of team status, self-regulation, and choking under pressure. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27, 97–106.
Jordet, G., Elferink-Gemser, M. T., Lemmink, K.A.P.M., & Visscher, C. (2006). The “Russian roulette” of soccer? Perceived control and anxiety in a major tournament penalty shootout. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 37, 281–298.
Jordet, G. & Hartman, E. (2008). Avoidance motivation and choking under pressure in soccer penalty shootouts. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 30, 450–457.
Jordet, G., Hartman, E., Visscher, C., & Lemmink, K.A.P.M. (2007). Kicks from the penalty mark in soccer: The roles of stress, skill, and fatigue for kick outcomes. Journal of Sports Sciences, 25, 121–129. doi: 10.1080/02640410600624020
Kane, M. J., Conway, A.R.A., Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2007). Variation in working memory as variation in executive attention and control. In A.R.A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. N. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 21–48). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2000). Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 336–358.
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47–70.
Kareev, Y. (2000). Seven (indeed, plus or minus two) and the detection of correlations. Psychological Review, 107, 397–403.
Kegelaers, J., & Wylleman, P. (2018). Exploring the coach’s role in fostering resilience in elite athletes. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 8, 239-254. http://dx.ddoi.org/10.1037/spy0000151
Klinger, E. (1996). The content of thoughts: Interference as the downside of adaptive normal mechanisms in thought flow. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference theories, methods, and findings (pp. 3–23). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Koedijker, J. M., Oudejans, R.R.D., & Beek, P. J. (2008). Table tennis performance following explicit and analogy learning over 10,000 repetitions. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 39, 237–256.
Land, M. F. (2009). Vision, eye movements, and natural behavior. Visual Neuroscience, 26, 51–62.
Laurin, R., & Finez, L. (2020) Working memory capacity does not always promote dual-task motor performance: The case of juggling in soccer. Scandinavian Journal of Pyschology, 61, 168-176. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12589
Lawrence, G. P., Cassell, V. E., Beattie, S., Woodman, T., Khan, M. A., Hardy, L., & Gottwald, V. M. (2014). Practice with anxiety improves performance, but only when anxious: evidence for the specificity of practice hypothesis. Psychological Research, 78, 634-650. doi: 10.1007/s00426-013-0521-9
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York, NY: Springer.
Leach, J., & Griffith, R. (2008). Restrictions in working memory capacity during parachuting: A possible cause of “no pull” fatalities. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 147–157.
Lee, T. G., Acuna, D. E., Kordling, K. P., Grafton, S. T. (2019). Limiting motor skill knowledge via incidental training protects against choking under pressure. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 279-290. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1486-x
Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk taking, and self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1250–1267.
Lewis, B. P., & Linder, D. E. (1997). Thinking about choking? Attentional processes and paradoxical performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 937–944.
Liao, C-M, & Masters, R. S. W. (2002). Self-focused attention and performance failure under psychological stress. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24, 289–305.
Liu, S. Boiangin, N., Meir, G., Shaffer, K. A., Lebeau, J-C., Basevitch, I., & Tenenbaum, G. (2019). Ironic and overcompensating processes under avoidance instructions in motor tasks: An attentional imbalance model with golf-putting evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 45, doi: 10.1037/t69599-000
Low, W. R., Sandercock, G. R. H., Butt, J., Freeman, P., Winter, M. E., & Maynard, I. (2021). Pressure training for performance domains: A meta-analysis. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 10, 149-163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/spy0000202
MacLeod, C. (1996). Anxiety and cognitive processes. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference theories, methods, and findings (pp. 47–76). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Macquet, A. C. (2009). Recognition within the decision-making process: A case study of expert volleyball players. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21, 64–79.
Mahoney, M. J., Gabriel, T. J., & Perkins, T. S. (1987). Psychological skills and exceptional athletic performance. The Sport Psychologist, 1, 181–199.
Markman, A. B., Maddox, W. T., & Worthy, D. A. (2006). Choking and excelling under pressure. Psychological Science, 17, 944–948.
Martens, R. (1977). Sport Competition Anxiety Test. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Martens, R. (1987). Coaches guide to sport psychology. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Martens, R., Burton, D., Vealey, R, Bump, L., & Smith, D. (1990). The development of the Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2. In R. Martens, R. S. Vealey, & D. Burton (Eds.), Competitive anxiety in sport (pp. 117–190). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Marty-Dugas, J., Smith, A. C., & Smilek, D. (2023). Focus on your breath: Can mindfulness facilitate the experience of flow? Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 10(3), 254–280. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000251
Masicampo, E. J., Luebber, F., & Baumeister, R. F. (2020). The influence of conscious thought is best observed over time. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 7(1), 87–102. https://doi-org.wcsu.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/cns0000205
Masters, R.S.W. (2000). Theoretical aspects of implicit learning in sport. International Journal of Sports Psychology, 31, 530–541.
Masters, R.S.W. (1992). Knowledge, nerves, and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 343–358.
Masters, R.S.W., Polman, R.C.J., & Hammond, N. V. (1993). “Reinvestment”: A dimension of personality implicated in skill breakdown under pressure. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 655–666.
McDaniel, K. D., Cummings, J. L., & Shain, S. (1989). The “yips”: A focal dystonia in golfers. Neurology, 39, 192–195.
McKay, J. M., Selig, S. E., Carlson, J. S., & Morris, T. (1997). Psychophysiological stress in elite golfers during practice and competition. The Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 29, 55–61.
Mellalieu, S. D., Hanton, S., & Fletcher, D. (2006). A competitive anxiety review: Recent and experience. Scandinavian Journal of Science & Medicine in Sport, 14, 326–334. directions in sport psychology research. In S. Hanton & S. D. Mellalieu (Eds.), Literature reviews in sport psychology. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.
Mellalieu, S. D., Hanton, S., & Jones, G. (2003). Emotional labeling and competitive anxiety in preparation and competition. The Sport Psychologist, 17, 157–174.
Mellalieu, S. D., Hanton, S., & O’Brien, M. (2004). Intensity and direction of competitive anxiety as a function of sport type. Scandanavian Journal of Medical Science in Sports, 14, 326-34. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2004.00389.x
Mellalieu, S. D., Hanton, S., & Thomas, O. (2009). The effects of a motivational general-arousal imagery intervention upon preperformance symptoms in male rugby union players. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 175–185. doi: 10.1016/j. psychsport.2008.07.003
Mellalieu, S. D., Neil, R., Hanton, S., & Fletcher, D. (2009). Competition stress in sport performers: Stressors experienced in the competition environment. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27, 729–744. doi: 10.1080/02640410902889834
Merced, M. (2018). The psychodynamics of “choking” under performance pressure. Practice Innovations. 4, 28-41. http:dx.doi.org/10.1037/pri0000080
Mesagno, C., Marchant, D., & Morris, T. (2008). A pre-performance routine to alleviate choking in “choking-susceptible” athletes. The Sport Psychologist, 22, 439–457.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magic number seven plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. 2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.
Moran, A. P. (2009). Attention, concentration and thought management. In B. W. Brewer (Ed.), Handbook of sports medicine and science, sport psychology (pp. 18–29). Chichester, England: Wiley-Blackwell.
Morgulev, E., & Galily, Y. (2018). Choking or delivering under pressure? The case of elimination games in NBA playoffs. Frontiers in Psychology. doi: 10.3389/psyg.201800979
Mullen, R., & Hardy, L. (2000). State anxiety and motor performance: Testing the conscious processing hypothesis. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18, 785–799.
Mullen, R., Hardy, L., & Tattersall, A. (2005). The effects of anxiety on motor performance: A test of the conscious processing hypothesis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 27, 212–225.
Mullen, R., Lane, A., & Hanton, S. (2009). Anxiety symptom interpretation in high-anxious, defensive high-anxious, low-anxious and repressor sport performers. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 22, 91–100. doi: 10.1080/10615800802203769
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259.
Murray, N. P., & Janelle, C. M. (2003). Anxiety and performance: A visual search examination of the processing efficiency theory. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25, 171–187.
Navarro, M., Miyamoto, N., van der Kamp, J., Morya, E., Ranvaud, R., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2012). The effects of high pressure on the point of no return in simulated penalty kicks. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 34, 83–101.
Nideffer, R. M. (1985). Athletes guide to mental training. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Nideffer, R. M. (1993). Attention control training. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphy, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 542–556). New York, NY: Macmillan.
Nideffer, R. M., & Sagal, M. (2006). Concentration and attention control training. In J. M. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal growth to peak performance (5th ed., pp. 382–403). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
Nideffer, R. M., Sagal, M-S., Lowry, M., & Bond, J. (2001). Identifying and developing world class performers. In G. Tenenbaum (Ed.), Reflections and experiences in sport and exercise psychology (pp. 129–144). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology.
Niering, M., Monsberger, T., Seifert, J., & Muehlbauer, T. (2023). Effects of psychological Interventions on performance anxiety in performing artists and athletes: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Behavioral Science, 13(910). https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13110910
Nieuwenhuys, A., Pijpers, J. R., Oudejans, R.R.D., & Bakker, F. C. (2008). The influence of anxiety on visual attention in climbing. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 30, 171–185.
Nieuwenhuys, A., Vos, L., Pijpstra, S., & Bakker, F. C. (2011). Meta experiences and coping effectiveness in sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 12, 135–143. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2010.07.008
Otten, M. (2009). Choking vs. clutch performance: A study of sport performance under pressure. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 31, 583–601.
Oudejans, R. R. D., Kuipers, W., Kooijman, C. C., & Bakker, F. C. (2011). Thoughts and attention of athletes under pressure: skill-focus of performance worries. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 24, 59-73. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2010.481331
Oudejans, R.R.D., & Pijpers, J. R. (2009). Training with anxiety has a positive effect on expert perceptual-motor performance under pressure. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1631–1647. doi: 10.1080/174702108025557702
Pain, M. A., & Harwood, C. G. (2008). The performance environment of the England youth soccer teams: A quantitative investigation. Journal of Sports Sciences, 26, 1157–1169. doi: 10.1080/02640410802101835
Perkins-Ceccato, N., Passmore, S. R., & Lee, T. D. (2003). Effects of focus of attention depend on golfers’ skill. Journal of Sports Sciences, 21, 593–600. doi:10.1080/0264041031000101980
Pété, E., Goisbault, M., Martinent, G., & Doron, J. (2023). Relationships between stress appraisal, coping, performance, and burnout among an artistic swimming team. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 12(3), 172–188. https://doi.org/10.1037/spy0000323
Petri, H. L., & Govern, J. (2004). Motivation: Theory, research, and applications (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.
Petrie, T. A. (1998). Anxiety management and the elite athlete: A case study. In K. F. Hayes (Ed.), Integrating exercise, sports, movement, and mind: Therapeutic unity (pp. 161–173). New York, NY: Haworth Press.
Pierce, G. R., Henderson, C. A., Yost, J. H., & Loffredo, C. M. (1996). Cognitive interference and personality: theoretical and methodological issues. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference theories, methods, and findings (pp. 285–296). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Pijpers, J. R., Oudejans, R.R.D., & Bakker, F. C. (2005). Anxiety-induced changes in movement behavior during the execution of a complex whole-body task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 421–445. Pijpers, J. R., Oudejans, R.R.D., Bakker, F. C., & Beek, P. J. (2006). The role of anxiety in perceiving and realizing affordances. Ecological Psychology, 18, 131–161. Proctor, R. W., & Dutta, A. (1995). Skill acquisition and human performance. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Raffety, B. D., Smith, R. E., & Ptacek, J. T. (1997). Facilitating and debilitating trait anxiety, situational anxiety, and coping with an anticipated stressor: A process analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 892–906.
Reeves, J. L., Tenenbaum, G., & Lidor, R. (2007). Choking in front of the goal: The effects of self-consciousness. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 5, 240–254.
Rosen, J. B., & Schulkin, J (1998). From normal fear to pathological anxiety. Psychological Review, 105, 325-350.
Runswick, O. R., Roca, A., Williams, A. M., Bezodis, N. E., & North, J. S. (2018). The effects of anxiety and situation-specific context on perceptual-motor skill: A multi-level investigation. Psychological Research, 82, 708-719. doi: 10.1007/s00326-017-0856-8
Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., & Beilock, S. L. (2009). Multiple social identities and stereotype threat: Imbalance, accessibility, and working memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 949-966.
Sanders, G. S., Baron, R. S., & Moore, D. L. (1978). Distraction and social comparison as mediators of social facilitation effects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 291–303.
Sarason, I. G., Pierce, G. R., & Sarason, B. R. (1996). Domains of cognitive interference. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference theories, methods, and findings (pp. 139–152). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Scanlan, T. K., & Lewthwaite, R. (1984). Social psychological aspects of competition for male youth sport participants: I. Predictors of competitive stress. Journal of Sport Psychology, 6, 208–226.
Scanlan, T. K., Lewthwaite, R., & Jackson, B. L. (1984). Social psychological aspects of competition for male youth sport participants: II. Predictors of performance outcomes. Journal of Sport Psychology, 6, 422–429.
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115, 336–356.
Schommer, N. C., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (2003). Dissociation between reactivity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system to repeated psychosocial stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 450–460.
Schlenker, B. R., Phillips, S. T., Boniecki, K. A., & Schlenker, D. R. (1995). Championship pressures: Choking or triumphing in one’s own territory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 632–643.
Schwarzer, R. (1996). Thought control of action: Interfering self-doubts. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference theories, methods, and findings (pp. 99–115). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Seve, C., Saury, J., Leblanc, S., & Durand, M. (2005). Course of action theory in table tennis: a qualitative analysis of the knowledge used by three elite players during matches. Revue Europeene de Psychologie Appliquee, 55, 145–155. doi: 10.1016/j. erap.2005.04.001
Seibt, B., & Förster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat and performance: How self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regulatory foci. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 38–56.
Singer, R. N. (2002). Preperformance state, routines, and automaticity: What does it take to realize expertise in self-paced events? Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24, 359–375.
Smith, N. C., Bellamy, M., Collins, D. J., & Newell, D. (2001). A test of processing efficiency theory in a team sport context. Journal of Sport Sciences, 19, 321–332.
Smith, R. E. (1996). Performance anxiety, cognitive interference, and concentration enhancement strategies in sports. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference theories, methods, and findings (pp. 261–283). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., Cumming, S. P., & Grossbard, J. R. (2006). Measurement of multidimensional sport performance anxiety in children and adults: The Sport Anxiety Scale-2. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 28, 479–501.
Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., & Passer, M. W. (2002). Sport performance anxiety in young athletes. In F. L. Smoll & R. E. Smith (Eds.), Children and youth in sport: A biosocial perspective (2nd ed.). Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.
Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., & Schultz, R. W. (1990). Measurement and correlates of sport-specific cognitive and somatic trait anxiety: The Sport Anxiety Scale. Anxiety Research, 2, 263–280.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Stinear, C. M., Coxon, J. P., Fleming, M. K., Lim, V. K., Prapavessis, H., Byblow, W. D. (2006). The yips in golf: Multimodal evidence for two subtypes. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 38, 1980–1989. doi: 10.1249/01. mss0000233792.93540.10
Tagler, M. J. (2012). Choking under the pressure of a positive stereotype: gender identification and self-conscious moderate men's math test anxiety. Journal of Social Psychology, 152, 401-416. Tauer, J. M., Guenther, C. L., & Rozek, C. (2009). Is there a home choke in decisive playoff basketball games? Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21, 148–162.
Tallis, F., & Eysenck, M. W. (1994). Worry: Mechanisms and modulating influences. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 22, 37–56.
Taylor, J. (1996). Intensity regulation and athletic performance. In J. L. Van Raalte & B. W. Brewer (Eds.), Exploring sport and exercise psychology (pp. 75–106). Washington, DC: APA.
Thompson, T., & Dinnell, D. L. (2007). Is self-worth protection best regarded as intentional self-handicapping behavior or an outcome of choking under pressure. Educational Psychology, 27, 509–531. doi: 1080/01443410601159910
Tod, D., Hardy, J., & Oliver, E. (2011). Effects of self-talk: A systematic review. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33(5). 666-687. doi: https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.33.5.666
van der Kamp, J., & Masters, R. S. W. (2008). The human Muller-Lyer illusion in goal-keeping. Perception, 37, 951–954. doi: 10.1068/p6010
van der Lei, H., & Tenenbaum, G. (2012). Performance processes within affect-related performance zones: A multi-modal investigation of golf performance. Applied Psychophysiological Biofeedback, 37, 229-240. doi: 10.1007/s10484-012-9195-0 Vast, R. L., Young, R. L., & Thomas, P. R. (2010). Emotions in sport: Perceived effects on attention, concentration, and performance. Australian Psychologist, 45, 132–140. doi: 10.1080/000500060903261538
Vereijken, B., van Emmerik, R.E.A., Whiting, H.T.A., & Newell, K. M. (1992). Free(z) ing degrees of freedom in skill acquisition. Journal of Motor Behavior, 23, 133–142.
Vickers, J. N. (1992). Gaze control in putting. Perception, 21, 117–132.
Vickers, J. N. (1996). Visual control when aiming at a far target. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 342–354.
Vickers, J. N., Rodrigues, S. T., & Edworthy, G. (2000). Quiet eye and accuracy in the dart throw. International Journal of Sports Vision, 6, 30–36.
Vickers, J. N., & Williams, A. M. (2007). Performing under pressure: The effects of physiological arousal, cognitive anxiety, and gaze control in biathlon. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39, 381–394.
Vine, S. J., & Wilson, M. R. (2010). Quiet eye training: Effects on learning and performance under pressure. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 22, 361–376. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2010.495106
Vorauer, J. D., & Turpie, C. A. (2004). Disruptive effects of vigilance on dominant group members’ treatment of outgroup members: Choking versus singing under pressure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 384–399. doi: 10.1037/0022– 3514.87.3.384
Wallace, H. M., Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2005). Audience support and choking under pressure: A home disadvantage? Journal of Sports Sciences, 23, 429–438.
Wang, J., Marchant, D., & Morris, T. (2004). Coping style and susceptibility to choking. Journal of Sport Behavior, 27, 75–92.
Wang, J., Marchant, D., Morris, T., & Gibbs, P. (2004). Self-consciousness and trait anxiety as predictors of choking in sport. Journal of Science in Medicine and Sport, 7, 174–185.
Wang, Z., & Shah, P. (2013). The effect of pressure on high- and low-working-memory students: An elaboration of the choking under pressure hypothesis. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 226-238.
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.
Wegner, D. M. (1997a). When the antidote is the poison: Ironic mental control processes. Psychological Science, 8, 148–150.
Wegner, D. M. (1997b). Why the mind wanders. In J. D. Cohen & J. W. Schooler (Eds.), Scientific approaches to consciousness (pp. 295–315). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wegner, D. M. (2011). Setting free the bears: Escape from thought suppression. American Psychologist, 66, 671–680.
Wegner, D. M., Broome, A., & Blumberg, S. J. (1997). Ironic effects of trying to relax under stress. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 35, 11–21.
Weierich, M. R., Treat, T. A., & Hollingworth, A. (2008). Theories and measurement of visual attentional processing in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 985–1018. doi: 10.1080/02699930701597601
Wells, B. M., & Skowronski, J. J. (2012). Evidence of choking under pressure on the PGA tour. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 34, 175-182. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2012.655629
Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 59–91.
Williams, A. M., Vickers, J., & Rodrigues, S. (2002). The effects of anxiety on visual search, movement kinematics, and performance in table tennis: A test of Eysenck and Calvo’s Processing Efficiency Theory. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24, 438–455.
Williams, S. E., Cumming, J., & Balanos, G. M. (2010). The use of imagery to manipulate challenge and threat appraisal states in athletes. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 32, 339–358.
Wilson, M. (2008). From processing efficiency to attentional control: A mechanistic account of the anxiety-performance relationship. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 184–201. doi: 10.1080/17509840802400787
Wilson, M., Chattington, M. Marple-Horvat, D. E., & Smith, N. C. (2007). A comparison of self-focused versus attentional explanations of choking. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 29, 439–456.
Wilson, M., & Smith, N. C. (2007). A test of the predictions of processing efficiency theory during elite team competition using the Thought Occurrence Questionnaire for Sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 38, 245–262.
Wilson, M., Smith, N., Chattington, M., Ford, M., & Marple-Horvat, D. E. (2006). The role of effort in moderating the anxiety-performance relationship: Testing the prediction of processing efficiency theory in simulated rally driving. Journal of Sports Sciences, 24, 1223–1233. doi: 10.1080/02640410500497667
Wilson, M., Smith, N. C., & Holmes, P. S. (2007). The role of effort in influencing the effect of anxiety on performance: Testing the conflicting predictions of processing efficiency theory and the conscious processing hypothesis. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 411–428. doi: 10.1348/000712606X133047
Wilson, M. R., Vine, S. J., & Wood, G. (2009). The influence of anxiety on visual attentional control in basketball free throw shooting. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 31, 152–168.
Wilson, M. R., Wood, G., & Vine, S. J. (2009). Anxiety, attentional control, and performance impairment in penalty kicks. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 31, 761–775.
Wilson, P. M., Mack, D. E., & Grattan, K. P. (2008). Understanding motivation for exercise: A self-determination theory perspective. Canadian Psychology, 49, 250–256. doi: 10.1037/a0012762
Winter, S., MacPherson, A. c. & Colling, D. (2014). "To think, or not to think, that is the question," Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 3, 102-115. doi: 10.1037/spy00000007 Wood, G., & Wilson, M. R. (2008). Gaze behavior and shooting strategies in football penalty kicks: Implications of a “keeper-dependent” approach. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 39, 1–18.
Wright, E. F., & Jackson, W. (1991) The home-course disadvantage in golf championships: Further evidence for the undermining effect of supportive audiences on performance under pressure. Journal of Sport Behavior, 14, 51–60.
Wright, E. F., & Voyer, D. (1995). Supporting audiences and performance under pressure: The home-ice disadvantage in hockey championships. Journal of Sport Behavior, 18, 21–28.
Wulf, G. (2007). Attention and motor skill learning. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Wulf, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Direction attention to movement effects enhances learning: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 648–660.
Wulf, G., & Su, J. (2007). An external focus of attention enhances golf shot accuracy in beginners and experts. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 384–389.
Wunderlich, F., Weigelt, M. Rein, R., & Memmert, D. (2021). PLoS One, 16, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248590
Yee, P. L., & Vaughan, J. (1996). Integrating cognitive, personality, and social approaches to cognitive interference and distractibility. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference theories, methods, and findings (pp. 77–97). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Yu, R. (2015). Choking under pressure: the neuropsychological mechanisms of incentive-induced performance decrements. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00019
Zeidner, M. (1994). Personal and contextual determinants of coping and anxiety in an evaluative situation: A prospective study. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 899–918.
The good news is that with insight, preparation, and perhaps coaching, performance anxiety is manageable and may be resolved. In the pages that follow, I will discuss anxiety and the pathways by which it disrupts performance. One pathway is by disrupting concentration or our ability to manage concentration and focus. Another pathway is by micromanagement and “dechunking” of highly-practiced and automated motor skills. We will consider the effects of pressure and audiences. Finally, we will consider how to mitigate and resolve performance anxiety.
Anxiety is species specific or part of the evolutionary endowment of humans and other species. It is adaptive or useful to the degree that it serves as an internal signal of current or future danger. This anxiety facilitates advance preparation for challenges and stressors. Thus, facilitative anxiety goads us to study, practice, and do what is necessary to perform optimal when the time arrives. When optimally prepared, anxiety is likely to lessen when the challenge begins. Anxiety is debilitative or disruptive when it interferes with this process of preparation to master sources of the stress. With debilitative anxiety we assume a defensive position as we try to protect ourselves and avoid the sources of threat. This of course is not adaptive when challenges – such as auditions, competitions, and performances – cannot be avoided (Anderson, 2021, Carver & Scheier, 1992; Gallucci, 2014; Jones & Hanton, 1996, 2001; Raffety, Smith, & Ptacek, 1997; Rosen & Schulkin, 1998, Tallis & Eysenck, 1994; Zeidner, 1994) .
Anxiety that is activated in response to specific situations, such as competitions and evaluations, is state anxiety, whereas anxiety levels that remain relatively stable across situations and over time is considered trait anxiety . Anxiety has cognitive and physiological manifestations. The physiological component consists of reactions of the sympathetic nervous system, such as muscle tension, elevated heart rate, sweating, and feelings of being keyed-up or on edge. The cognitive aspect consists of unbidden, intrusive, and persistent forms of worry. Cognitive anxiety is also referred to as cognitive interference because it literally captures, hijacks and commandeers our limited and impermanent attentional resources. Worries are often so emotionally valent or charged that they are difficult to ignore.
In everyday life, it is adaptive to experience anxiety and fear when faced with real danger. Anxiety motivates us to “drop everything” and avoid or escape danger . It is therefore not so easy to suppress thoughts of imagined danger such as “I’m not as good as the others” when performing under pressure. However, focusing on sources of imagined danger has the ironic effect of leading us to, rather than away, from what we fear . For example, worries about being inferior to others may lead you to an inhibited, muted performance that lacks force, texture and color. Focusing on what you wish to avoid is a profound source of cognitive interference because it divides your attention between what you wish to accomplish and what want to avoid. Under pressure, anxiety becomes more emotionally valent or charged, and you may think primarily of what you fear. Thus, if not prepared to manage pressure, cognitive interference may sweep aside our most carefully crafted game plans and beset us with thoughts of failure (Anderson, 2021; Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970; Wegner, 1994, 1997a, 1997b, 2011).
In a formal sense, the focus of our attention is the contents of our working memory. In a seminal pa per in 1956, George Miller revealed that the span of working memory was limited to seven (plus or minus two) units of information and that storage was temporary. Information is retained in working memory only until it is pushed out by other information. We concentrate when we deliberately manage the contents of our working memory by focusing of certain thoughts and excluding others. More on Working Memory
We are eminently distractible. The average college student shifts the content of his or her thoughts 4,000 times in the course of one day. Attention shifts whenever a person encounters an environmental cue or has a thought that arouses emotion because of its relation to their current concerns or what is considered important.
Cognitive interference disrupts concentration by intruding on one’s planned focus – the keys to successful performance. There simply is not room for both in working memory. Worries may be difficult to ignore because they are emotionally valent or charged. Emotionally valent thoughts often pertain to potential danger such as: “will I be successful, don’t make a mistake, I’m going to embarrass myself, and I don’t think I can do it.” This self-focused cognitive anxiety also provokes physiological anxiety. Cognitive Interference as a Kibitizer and Schnorrer
Optimal performance requires immersion in the present moment, whereas cognitive interference takes up out of the moment. Worries are often focused on the future and self-stereotypes are backward-looking.
Negative self-stereotypes are a form of habitual and automatic self-talk such as “I don’t perform well under pressure.” With this assumption, performers approach competition with the dread of confirming the self-stereotype. They are vigilant for signs of choking. Of course, this vigilance commanders working memory storage space that is critically limited and essential for optimal performance. Ironically, vigilance for signs of choking leads to choking because attention is diverted from the second-to-second process of the actual performance and directed to whether performance confirms stereotypes or expectations. Most of us would hardly find it flattering to be identified as one who stereotypes others. And yet, we may blithely stereotype ourselves.
Some performers worry that they will fall short of their high standards. They experience cognitive interference as they compare their performance to positive self-stereotypes rather than giving their attention entirely to the moment. Worry may extend to not meeting the high standards of audiences. This is especially important when performers very much want to gain the admiration of an audience and lack confidence that they will do so. Self-stereotypes are handicapping because they position one in a defensive posture of attempting to avoid failure.
In the sections above, cognitive interference and distraction has been described as “normal” or universal. However, in addition, some readers may be especially vulnerable to self-critical and harshly judgmental self-talk. This may be the result of regular exposure to harsh and critical parents, mentors, and coaches. Quite unconsciously, this critical focus may become internalized so that it becomes the child’s own. Unconsciously and unintentionally, the child then filters experience through a self-critical bias, and being “hard on myself” becomes one’s “normal.” This harshly judgmental bias is meta-cognitive, or a ubiquitous manner of processing information and viewing ourselves. Its recognition and management may therefore require concerted and prolonged effort. Exploring the origins of dysfunctional self-talk may be threating and destabilizing as it challenges one’s view of self and others that was established in primary attachment relationships.
Given that people with trait anxiety are almost always anxious, the effect of pressure on their performance is magnified. They are acutely sensitive to signs of danger and threat and of course cognitive interference. They may assume that failure is inevitable when faced with even minimal difficulties and impediments. Pessimism comes easily and they may give up before even starting (Conroy, 2008; Hiroaki, Maruo, Meyer, Hajak, 2017; Petri & Govern, 2004).
Some performers are affected less by cognitive interference than by a self-conscious focus on their performance. These performances look “tight” and “stiff” and lack expressiveness and emotional tone. Some are overly deliberative and others are rushed. They micromanage skills that they have practiced and mastered to such a degree that they best allowed to “run” automatically and without conscious thought about their component parts. This process has been studied exhaustively in sport psychology and is referred to as explicit monitoring or conscious processing (Beilock & Gray, 2007; Masters, 1992). Freezing Under Pressure
Complex motor, vocal, and musical skills are often deconstructed in the learning process. Beginners practice the component parts and integrate the parts into an omnibus unit that can be executed automatically and as the unit, when mastered. When anxiety prompts performers to consciously monitor or form conscious mental images of the step-by-step components and processes that make up automated units, performance becomes stilted, awkward, error-prone, and more characteristic of a beginner rather than an expert. This process is ironically referred to as “reinvestment” as dividends are scarce.Not Mere Metaphores
The disruptive effects of anxiety generally worsen with increased pressure. Pressure refers to the conditions or factors that increase the importance of performing well (Gallucci, 2014). When performance is most important, in the presence of an evaluating audience, when competition is stiff, and when there is only one chance to be successful, pressure is highest. Choking is a term that is widely recognized to refers to inferior performances under pressure. Performances that do not measure up to a person’s acquired skill level are considered inferior. Choking is paradoxical because performers are often most highly motivated to do their best when the stakes or consequences of performances are highest. However, choking does not occur as a result of insufficient effort, but rather because pressure impairs skilled performance by flooding working memory with cognitive interference (Runswick, Roca, Williams, Bezodis, & North, 2018). The Physiology of Distraction
1. Insight is key. Cognitive interference and behavior micromanagement can occur automatically and unconsciously. Without understanding, you may believe your thoughts are uncontrollable and that you are helpless. With insight, the mystery recedes, and you can set yourself to the task of managing your thoughts and emotions.
2. Make a commitment to managing your thoughts. Absorption in the moment is the goal and thoughts of the future and the past are cognitive interference. To the degree possible, occupy your working memory with your game plan and what you intend to do at each moment of competition. When you focus on your game plan, you manage your thoughts from the “top-down.” If you do not take charge of your thoughts from the top-down, chances are that your focus will be hijacked from the “bottom-up” by distractions from opponents, observers, and your own thoughts. When you notice your thoughts being captured from the bottom-up, interrupt the distractions and return to a top-down focus: Stop! Back to my plan. (Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., & Beilock, S. L. (2009). Multiple social identities and stereotype threat: Imbalance, accessibility, and working memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 949-966.
3. Furthermore, when you focus on what you intend to accomplish, you lessen the chances of the self-conscious micromanagement of skills that “dechunks” automated skills and erodes performance. According to the common-coding theory, a focus on intended action engages sensory neuronal tracks that are adjacent to motor neuronal tracks in the cortex. The sensory track primes and elicits the sequence of motor skills necessary to produce those intended results. Stay in tempo. Notice when you are overly deliberative and cautious and when you are rushing. Develop cue words to invoke omnibus routines and automated skills such as rhythm, tempo, and just like always. (Beckmann, J., Gröpel, P., Ehrlenspiel, F. (2013). Preventing motor skill failure through hemisphere-specific priming: Cases from choking under pressure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 679-691. doi: 10.1037/a0029852)
4. Be prepared for lapses. If your mind wanders to think of the worst that might happen, be prepared to answer with something to the effect of so what! If you notice a burst of anxiety, engage in a practice such as centering to return to homeostasis.
Let your mind rest on the experience of strength and balance. Briefly direct your attention inward to check your pattern of breathing and muscular tension. Do you notice shallow, thoracic breathing, anxiety and muscle tension? Replace thoracic breathing with deep, abdominal breathing.
It is recommended for use just prior to crucial points in a performance or competition when anxiety is likely to be highest. Its practice is also recommended during natural pauses in competitions, as well as prior to and after performances.
Centering has the secondary benefit of interrupting cognitive interference. From the standing position, athletes are directed to spread their legs to shoulder width and to flex slightly at the knees. It is a technique for gaining momentary control over physiological anxiety or arousal and concentration.
Characteristics of centering are similar to those of the relaxation response. Both emphasize attention to breathing and techniques are straightforward. Centering offers the considerable promise of being maximally “portable,” in that it can be practiced before, after, and during competitions.
5. Recognize if you are prone to trait anxiety and self-critical, judgmental self-talk. These traits make it more difficult to be entirely in the moment and focus on task execution. If you wish to cling to judgmental self-talk and consider it necessary to motivate hard work and avoid laziness, try to relegate it until after performances and competitions are completed. At that point it will have less effect on your performance than your happiness.
6. Learn from your experience and do not stereotype yourself with self-talk such as I don’t audition well. Your past difficulties are situational and not necessarily predictive of your future.
7. We often habituate or learn to manage anxiety as a result of experience with live auditions and performances. Pressure-training or practice with simulated pressure also provides opportunities to learn to perform optimally when experiencing anxiety. Without this experience, the skills you performed flawlessly in the sanctuary of your home is out of reach when you step on stage. Seek out opportunities to perform under pressure and demonstrate to yourself that your performance holds up under pressure. For example, can you dance on a stage that is cramped and poorly lit? Can you sing on key with the noise of a generator droning in the background? Are you prepared to perform with boorish oafs and before unruly audiences? Does your efficacy shrink when you are faced with stronger competition? By familiarizing and preparing for adversity, you inoculate yourself against anxiety by giving yourself proof that you can still deliver. You also learn new mental and physical skills for managing adversity – you build resiliency and mental toughness.
8. Perhaps you do not have resilient self-esteem and therefore readily doubt yourself. While these dispositions may result from childhood experiences and are not readily reversed, you may nevertheless immerse yourself in the moment of performance. As adults and adolescents, we cannot readily wrest the insouciance that is a derivative of the gleam in the mother’s eye, but we can recognize critical self-judgments as mere thoughts and be prepared to answer, so what!
9. Stick with elements of your performance that you know will hold up under the most intense pressure. Efficacy or certainty that you can execute all the elements in your performance is a powerful source of insulation against cognitive interference. If possible, avoid stretching to reach skills outside of your range. It is difficult to avoid worry if you attempt to belt notes that result in voice cracks 50% of the time.
10. Practice self-talk that affirms efficacy. When facing difficult challenges, I suggest the motivational self-talk of I will do this. You can control the quality of your preparation and effort to apply these recommendations to perform to the best of your ability. Thoughts about controlling outcomes are but another form of cognitive interference.
11. Self-efficacy is not built upon wishful thinking. The most convincing source of efficacy is your actual performance. Are you optimally prepared and conditioned? Have you left nothing to chance in preparing for your performance? Have you checked to make sure you have packed everything? Did you arrive at the venue with ample time to familiarize yourself with the venue, invite “butterflies” and then allow anxiety to dissipate as you habituate to the venue and feel more comfortable? Are you able to see your butterflies as normal signs of excitement rather than symptoms of debilitative anxiety? Have you gathered as much information as possible about what to expect with a new venture such as an audition and are you prepared for a change in plans? Do you have a practice for inducing relaxation and managing physiological anxiety such as centering or mindfulness? Are you prepared to fully inhabit a role, song, or dance and immerse yourself in the moment without judgment and concern about the responses of the audience?
12. The responses of audiences are so very important to performers. Ironically, their admiration is less likely when you focus on pleasing them as this is cognitive interference. To the degree possible, embody your part without a thought about their reactions. Take the lead and let the audience follow. They will let you know how they liked you later.
12a. When you have time, take a step back and examine your motivation for achievement. Ironically, some very achievement-oriented people are primarily motivated by the fear of failure. A focus on avoiding failure and mistakes is yet another form of cognitive interference. Avoiding failure is more likely with a focus on what you intend to do or achieve.